Synesthesia
is a neurological trait possessed by approximately 4% of the general
population.
For these
people, perceiving a certain stimulus (sensory or conceptual) involuntarily and
consistently triggers a second perceptual experience, typically via a different
sense.
There
are many different types, and people who have synesthesia (called synesthetes)
usually have several at once.
The
most frequent type is coloured sequence synesthesia, where elements forming
part of learnt sequences of concepts such as letters, numbers, days or months evoke a colour perception. Grapheme-colour synesthesia, in
which each letter and/or number is associated with its corresponding colour, is
the most common type of coloured sequence synesthesia. Approximately 60% of
synesthetes have this kind.
Auditory-visual
synesthesia is another relatively common type, as about a third of all
synesthetes have it. These are the people who can “see
music”: sounds, timbres, musical notes or songs trigger visual perceptions. There
are various subtypes of auditory-visual synesthesia: while some people see or
feel a different colour on hearing each individual musical note, others get their synesthetic perceptions from timbre, visualising different geometric shapes for
example for the sound of each musical instrument, while for other synesthetes
general day-to-day sounds evoke colour, shape, position and movement. In
fact, no two cases are alike.
Another
relatively common type of synesthesia is spatial sequence, with subtypes
including calendar synesthesia, where the concept of time units – hours, days,
months, years, decades and so on – are perceived visually on a kind of mental
map around them, which can shift its perspective depending on the current day
or month, etc.
Other
types of synesthesia clearly involve two different senses, such as
olfactory-visual synesthesia, where smells evoke visual perceptions (colours
and/or shapes), or auditory-tactile, where hearing certain sounds (those of
different musical instruments, for example) consistently triggers tactile
sensations on different parts of the body.
Some
synesthetes see their colours or other synesthetic concurrents physically as if
they were on a “screen” in front of them, while others (the vast majority) only
perceive them in the mind’s eye or “know” they are there. The
first type of synesthetes are called projectors or are said to have projective
synesthesia, and the latter type are associators or have associative
synesthesia. In both cases the visual concurrent that is seen or
perceived is called a photism.
Synesthesia
always involves an inducer and a concurrent. The inducer is the
specific stimulus that triggers the synesthetic experience – hearing a certain
musical note, for example. The concurrent is the additional perception that is
triggered, which might be feeling the sensation of the colour dark blue when
that specific note is heard. Other features of synesthesia are
that the experiences are consciously perceived; involuntary or automatic; idiosyncratic
(the same inducers are associated with different concurrents in each
synesthete); consistent; memorable; and usually pleasant, with the vast
majority of synesthetes enjoying the way they perceive their world. It
is also true that their form of perception seems totally normal to them and
they are often deeply shocked when they realise that most people do not see the
world like they do. Of course, many of them have no
idea that they have such a thing as synesthesia. But it’s never too late
to realise and the “eureka moment” can come at any age.
Are
synesthetes born or made? They’re born. Synesthesia
is a hereditary trait and synesthetes are often aware of other family members
who have it too. In 2018 some of the genetic variants
responsible for the phenomenon were identified, although it seems clear that many
different genes are involved, most of them still unknown. It
is also a fact that not all people born with these genes actually express them,
so there are even cases of identical twins where one is a synesthete and the
other is not. There are also people with a very high
“synesthetic disposition” – multiple types, strongly expressed – and others
with a lower disposition, who might have only one or two types and only mild
experiences with them. Synesthesia is rather more common
among people on the autism spectrum (it has been estimated that as compared to
a frequency of around 4% in the general population, 15%-20% of those people on the
spectrum could have it). Up until about 15 years ago
synesthesia was thought to be much more common in women than in men, but
today’s rigorous study methods have disproven this theory, discovering that it
is actually equally prevalent in both sexes. Another theory that has
bitten the dust is that there is a higher incidence of left-handedness in the
synesthete population: it is now known that this is not the case, although
there do seem to be more ambidextrous or cross-dominant people than in the
general population (although still a minority).
With regard to the neurological basis for synesthesia, the main theories that have been put forward are 1. the cross-activation (“neural pruning”) theory (Ramachandran and Hubbard, 2001), 2. the disinhibited feedback theory (Neufeld et al., 2012) and, recently, 3. the stochastic resonance model (Lalwani and Brang, 2019). None of these models have yet been conclusively proven as the definitive basis, and research is ongoing. There is a brief description of each theory on this page, with links to the original studies and further reading.
On
the Tree website we only talk about developmental or natural synesthesia, which is
present from birth and is the result of genetic expression, although induced or
acquired synesthesia also exists, producing experiences that are similar to
(but not exactly the same as) those of congenital synesthesia and which can be
caused by brain injury, drug use – mainly hallucinogens – or other less common
causes.
While the first studies on synesthetic phenomena date back to the early nineteenth century, it was only in the 1990s that more mainstream research began to be dedicated to synesthesia and the general population became more aware of it. Some relevant researchers are Richard Cytowic, Vilyanur Ramachandran, Sean Day, Julia Simner, Jamie Ward, Simon Baron-Cohen, Lawrence Marks, David Eagleman and Anton Dorso, among others. Conferences and congresses are held worldwide, bringing together researchers, synesthetes and other people with an interest in the subject, and there are an increasing number of online resources for anyone who wants to read recent research, find out basic information and make contact or compare experiences with other synesthetes.
Articles for further reading
This recent scientific article (November 2021) by Jamie Ward gives a brilliant comprehensive overview of synesthesia in the state of the art, addressing all its main aspects.
Here's a recent general article on synesthesia I can also recommend, by Sydney Perkowitz for Nautilus magazine.
If you're looking for a readable, easy-to-understand article about synesthesia that is fun while being scientifically correct, you'll like this one by Clare Jonas on her website That Thinking Feeling.
Here's another good article that touches on many different aspects of synesthesia: Exploring Synesthesia: A Journey into Neuroscience of Perception by Shelly Jones on the site Webmedy (2023).
In this video, "Synaesthesia Masterclass", Dr. Mary Spiller, a cognitive psychologist and lecturer at the University of East London, gives a good overview of what synesthesia is and what kind of tests can be done to determine it.
... and in a recent podcast in her Let's Talk Synaesthesia series, Maike Preissing interviewed leading scientist and synesthesia researcher Julia Simner, who described what it's all about. You can listen via this link and if you click on the Transcription tag you can also see an (automatically generated) written transcription too.
This page last updated: 05 May 2024
This page is All About Synaesthesia
No comments:
Post a Comment